The Classification of Languages 195 VIII DRAVIDIAN (a) Tamil (ft) Telugu (c) Canarese IX BANTU Kafir, Swahih, Bechuana, Sesuto, Hereto, Congo, Ditala, etc GRAMMATICAL CHARACTERISTICS OF LANGUAGE-FAMILIES Because grammatical similarities between different languages furnish one of the three most important indications of evolutionary relation- ship, it is useful to recognize certain general grammatical features which may be more or less characteristic of a language From this point of view we can classify language-types which may coincide with genuine evolutionary affinity, if the evidence of grammar is supported by other clues such as the two already discussed If other clues are not available3 the fact that languages are classified in this way does not necessarily point to common ongin, because languages which are related may have lost outstanding grammatical similarities, and languages which belong to different families may have evolved similar grammatical traits along different paths Fiom this pomt of view, we can divide languages into the following types —isolating, flexional, root-inflected and dassificatory The first and the last are the most cleai-cut, and the second, which embraces a great diversity of tongues, depends on grammatical devices which have no common origin Even when we stretch the limits of all three to the utmost, we are left with many languages in which isolated flexional and dassificatory features may be blended without decisive predominance of any one of them, and the language of a single com- munity may traverse the boundaries of such groups in a comparatively short period of its history. Thus the English of Alfred the Great was a typically flexional language, and Anglo-American is predominantly isolating Basque, which is a law unto itself, the Amerindian dialects, and the speech of the Esquimaux in Greenland, fit into no clearly defined family based on evidence of common ancestry, and we cannot classify them in any of the three grammatical groups mentioned above. The word of an isolating language is an unalterable unit Neither fkxional accretions nor internal changes reveal what part the word plays in the sentence, as do the changes from house to houses, men to men'$9 give to gave> live to lived All the words which we should call verbs are fixed like must (p 123), and all the words we call nouns are fixed like grouse. Vernaculars of the Chinese family, usually cited as extreme examples of the isolating type, have other common features which are not necessarily connected with the fact that the word is an